Monday, July 31, 2006

Robotto Kânibaru


Encephalon - A neuroscience carnival

Third edition available at
Thinking Meat.




Read about human-robot communication.















Robot Carnival

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Friday, July 28, 2006

The Joy of Melanocortin Receptors


Lose Weight and Improve Your Sex Life!

Those two pitches account for approximately 95% of all spam, right? But what if you could manufacture one drug that promises to do both things? Think of the profits!!

Although it sounds too good to be true, there may yet be such a substance: drugs acting as agonists at the melanocortin 4 receptor (MC4-R). A recent article in Neuron focused on the weight loss aspects of MC4-R. Serotonergic agents were administered to obese and regular mice; both groups reduced their food intake as a result. The figure above illustrates how serotonin is thought to act on melanocortin pathways (via actions at the serotonin 1B receptor, 5-HT1BR).
Detailed explanation [Figure 8 of Heisler et al.]: 5-HT hyperpolarizes and inhibits AgRP neurons and decreases an inhibitory drive onto POMC cells by activation of 5-HT1BRs. 5-HT also activates POMC neurons via activation of 5-HT2CRs. This leads to reciprocal increases in a-MSH release and decreases in AgRP release at MC4-R in target sites.
Translated, the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus may play a central role in appetite suppression. The arcuate has two populatons of neurons: one expressing the anorectic melanocortin receptor agonist, a-MSH (green neuron) and the other expressing the appetitite-stimulating melanocortin receptor antagonist, AgRP (blue neuron). The combined increase in a-MSH and decrease in AgRP act at downstream MC4 receptors to suppress appetitite.
Heisler LK, Jobst EE, Sutton GM, Zhou L, Borok E, Thornton-Jones Z, Liu HY, Zigman JM, Balthasar N, Kishi T, Lee CE, Aschkenasi CJ, Zhang CY, Yu J, Boss O, Mountjoy KG, Clifton PG, Lowell BB, Friedman JM, Horvath T, Butler AA, Elmquist JK, Cowley MA. (2006). Serotonin Reciprocally Regulates Melanocortin Neurons to Modulate Food Intake. Neuron 51:239-249.

The neural pathways through which central serotonergic systems regulate food intake and body weight remain to be fully elucidated. We report that serotonin, via action at serotonin1B receptors (5-HT(1B)Rs), modulates the endogenous release of both agonists and antagonists of the melanocortin receptors, which are a core component of the central circuitry controlling body weight homeostasis. We also show that serotonin-induced hypophagia requires downstream activation of melanocortin 4, but not melanocortin 3, receptors. These results identify a primary mechanism underlying the serotonergic regulation of energy balance and provide an example of a centrally derived signal that reciprocally regulates melanocortin receptor agonists and antagonists in a similar manner to peripheral adiposity signals.
However, the serotonergic drugs given to the mice in that study included D-fenfluramine, part of the scary and dangerous fen-phen fiasco. Fen-phen was linked to heart valve disease and pulmonary hypertension, and was withdrawn from the U.S. market in 1997. The mice were also given an experimental 5-HT1BR agonist not yet approved for human use.

Are there any drugs that act directly at MC4-R? And what about sexual function? Where is that miracle aphrodisiac weight loss supplement you promised??

In contrast to the modest weight loss associated with short-term SSRI treatment, these antidepressant drugs are associated with high rates of sexual side effects. Enter PT-141.

Diamond LE, Earle DC, Rosen RC, Willett MS, Molinoff PB. (2004). Double-blind, placebo-controlled evaluation of the safety, pharmacokinetic properties and pharmacodynamic effects of intranasal PT-141, a melanocortin receptor agonist, in healthy males and patients with mild-to-moderate erectile dysfunction. Int J Impot Res. 16:51-9.

Rosen RC, Diamond LE, Earle DC, Shadiack AM, Molinoff PB. (2004). Evaluation of the safety, pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamic effects of subcutaneously administered PT-141, a melanocortin receptor agonist, in healthy male subjects and in patients with an inadequate response to Viagra. Int J Impot Res. 16:135-42.

Diamond LE, Earle DC, Heiman JR, Rosen RC, Perelman MA, Harning R. (2006). An effect on the subjective sexual response in premenopausal women with sexual arousal disorder by bremelanotide (PT-141), a melanocortin receptor agonist. J Sex Med. 3:628-38.


Back in April, there was an article in The Observer Magazine and a post in Pharyngula about this compound. A good place to start if you really want to learn about MC4-R agonists is this article:

Nargund RP, Strack AM, Fong TM. (2006). Melanocortin-4 receptor (MC4R) agonists for the treatment of obesity. J Med Chem. 49:4035-43.

Funniest lines from the article:
If MC4R agonists induce spontaneous penile erections in men, this would represent a significant impediment to the development of compounds to treat obesity.
The nonselective melanocortin agonists, 1 and 3, have nausea and vomiting as adverse side effects when administered to humans either subcutaneously or intranasally. Attempts to study whether the effects of these two structurally related molecules are mechanism-based have been of limited utility.
Is PT-141 really a miracle drug? That's highly doubtful. But, as PZ Myers said,
Wow. Makes me want to run out and buy stock in Palatin Technologies, the manufacturer.

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Wednesday, July 26, 2006

Spindle Neurons: The Next New Thing?

In a neuroanatomical tour de force, Nimchinsky and colleagues (1999) obtained access to samples of the anterior cingulate cortex (and other cortical regions) from 28 different primate species, from prosimians to anthropoids to great apes to humans. They processed the samples with a Nissl stain to identify neuronal cell bodies in the cerebral cortex, a structure that (generally) consists of six layers. Spindle neurons are a unique type of neuron found in layer Vb in the ACC and frontoinsular cortex of humans. This is nothing new; spindle neurons (also called Von Economo neurons) were first identified in the 19th century by W. Betz (of the eponymous Betz cell fame, I presume) and by Nobel laureate Santiago Ramón y Cajal. What was new in 1999 was the finding that only humans and great apes have spindle neurons.

Nimchinsky EA, Gilissen E, Allman JM, Perl DP, Erwin JM, Hof PR. (1999). A neuronal morphologic type unique to humans and great apes. Proc Natl Acad Sci 96:5268-73.

We report the existence and distribution of an unusual type of projection neuron, a large, spindle-shaped cell, in layer Vb of the anterior cingulate cortex of pongids and hominids. These spindle cells were not observed in any other primate species or any other mammalian taxa, and their volume was correlated with brain volume residuals, a measure of encephalization in higher primates. These observations are of particular interest when considering primate neocortical evolution, as they reveal possible adaptive changes and functional modifications over the last 15-20 million years in the anterior cingulate cortex, a region that plays a major role in the regulation of many aspects of autonomic function and of certain cognitive processes. That in humans these unique neurons have been shown previously to be severely affected in the degenerative process of Alzheimer's disease suggests that some of the differential neuronal susceptibility that occurs in the human brain in the course of age-related dementing illnesses may have appeared only recently during primate evolution.


Here's what John Allman's Lab at Cal Tech says about their work:
Our lab has investigated the anatomical structure of the Von Economo (spindle) neurons in anterior cingulate and fronto-insular cortex. Based on functional imaging studies of these brain areas and our studies of the expression of neurotransmitter receptors on these cells, we think they participate in fast, intuitive social decision-making. We have found that the Von Economo neurons emerge mainly in the first three years after birth. We also have evidence that in autistic subjects the Von Economo neurons are abnormally located, possibly as a result of a migration defect. This abnormality may be at least partially responsible for defective social intuition in autism.
Somehow, the "spindle neuron" meme hasn't caught on like the "mirror neuron" meme. Is it because spindle neurons have been only been described anatomically (not physiologically), while the reverse is true for mirror neurons? Anatomically speaking, do we know much about mirror neurons? Here's what Rizzolatti and Craighero (2004) have to say about them:
Mirror neurons are a particular class of visuomotor neurons, originally discovered in area F5 of the monkey premotor cortex, that discharge both when the monkey does a particular action and when it observes another individual (monkey or human) doing a similar action (Di Pellegrino et al. 1992, Gallese et al. 1996, Rizzolatti et al. 1996a).

from Rizzolatti G, Craighero L. (
2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci. 27:169-92.
In the elegantly titled article, The importance of being agranular, Stewart Shipp reviews evidence that approximately 10% of recorded cells in premotor area F5 in the macaque monkey can be classified as mirror neurons. He also points out an interesting conundrum regarding the anatomical organization of motor cortex: it's agranular, meaning it's lacking the granule cell layer (layer IV), the typical termination point for feedforward sensory information. Area 7b (or PF) in the rostral inferior parietal lobule provides the main parietal input to F5. Without going into too many details, it seems the anatomical circuitry of visual input to F5 is pretty complicated. Anyone who studies mirror neurons (or who does fMRI studies of "empathy and the mirror neuron system") should read these two papers:
Geyer S, Matelli M, Luppino G, Zilles K. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of the primate isocortical motor system. Anat Embryol 202:443-74

Shipp S. (2005). The importance of being agranular: a comparative account of visual and motor cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 360:797-814.


Gap ad, with mirror neurons by Rizzolatti & Craighero (2004)


Everybody's talking about mirror neurons!!


Con: Mixing Memory and Neurotopia (version 2.0)

Mirror Neurons, Language, and Meaning (Oh, My!)

Everybody Post About Mirror Neurons!!!


Less Con: The Frontal Cortex

Are Mirror Neurons Too Cool?


Pro: Small Gray Matters

Mirror neurons aren’t really all that bad…


ADDENDUM: and there's more!

Neurofuture: Mirror meme

Mind Hacks: Reflected glory

And the post that really started it all,

BrainTechSci: Much Ado About Mirror Neurons

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Monday, July 24, 2006

The "Coffee and Brain" Series

And view the other content at KERBLOG...


coffee and brain




coffee and brain 2




coffee and brain 3




copyright issues, from Mazen (the artist):
a lot of people are asking me if they can post this or that drawing on their blog, or to publish it, etc.

to all the blog readers and beyond,

PLEASE NOTE THAT THERE IS NO COPYRIGHT ON ANY OF THE DRAWINGS OF THIS BLOG. PLEASE DO PUBLISH, PHOTOCOPY, PRINT, DISTRIBUTE EVERYWHERE ALL THE DRAWINGS IF YOU CAN.

when the war is over, my lawyer will call you. until then please spread them anywhere you can.

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Carnival of Souls



The Synapse No. 3 (a neuroscience carnival) is now available for your reading pleasure at The Neurophilosopher's Blog!

July 23, 2006
The Haunting
By JOHN HODGMAN
New York Times Magazine
. . .

Horror, like comedy, has always been something of a reptilian-brain endeavor, unusual among the arts insofar as it is successful only when it is able to produce a single, audible emotional effect — a scream or a laugh — that is primal, cathartic and difficult to understand. This is one reason that horror has always been a director's medium: the horror movie is a contraption, and it takes a certain organizational flair to design, pace and frame a scare.


We've seen fMRI studies of neural responses to viewing happy and sad film clips, why not of horror films? The July 2005 issue of the International Journal of Psychophysiology is on the Neurobiology of Fear and Disgust.
Stark R, Schienle A, Sarlo M, Palomba D, Walter B, Vaitl D. (2005). Influences of disgust sensitivity on hemodynamic responses towards a disgust-inducing film clip. Int J Psychophysiol. 57(1):61-7.

The major goal of the present functional magnetic resonance imaging study was to investigate the influence of disgust sensitivity on hemodynamic responses during disgust induction. Fifteen subjects viewed three different film excerpts (duration: 135 s each) with disgust-evoking, threatening and neutral content. The films were presented in a block design with four repetitions of each condition. Afterwards, subjects gave affective ratings for the films and answered the questionnaire for the assessment of disgust sensitivity (QADS, []). The subjects' overall disgust sensitivity was positively related to their experienced disgust, as well as to their prefrontal cortex activation during the disgust condition. Further, there was a positive correlation between subjects' scores on the QADS subscale spoilage/decay and their amygdala activation (r=0.76). This was reasonable since the disgust film clip depicted a cockroach-invasion and the subscale spoilage/decay contains, among others, an item asking for disgust towards cockroaches. The study stresses, in accordance to previous studies, the importance of considering personality traits when studying affective responses in fMRI studies.


View trailer for Carnival of Souls.

For those into such things, there's a good analysis of that horror flick By PAUL KESLER.
Fundamentally, Carnival Of Souls is a visual exploration of death, which, in the course of this exploration, sees death from a subjective point of view that is at times reminiscent of Carl Dreyer's "Vampyr". However, its subjectivity is far more radical than that of the Dreyer film, since Dreyer, concentrating to some extent on actual occurences rooted in a particular time and place, dwelt only sporadically on the inner consciousness of his protagonist. By contrast, it could be argued that everything in Carnival Of Souls other than the physical death of the main character is subjective in nature. As a result, we are forced into suspension of disbelief as to the reality-status of a woman who, though materially dead, continues to experience many things through "normal" consciousness.
In the study of Stark et al. (2005),
Three film clips (DISGUST, FEAR, NEUTRAL, 135 s each) with sound track were presented to the subjects. Each clip was a scene selected from a commercial movie: DISGUST showed an invasion of cockroaches, a scene from the movie Creepshow I (Romero and Rubinstein, 1982). FEAR was drawn from The Hitcher (Harmon et al., 1986) and depicted a boy threatened by a man armed with a knife. NEUTRAL came from Switzerland—The Alpine Wonderland (Scro et al., 1989) and showed urban areas.
The paper was mostly about disgust and how the participants' self-ratings on a disgust sensitivity questionnaire correlated with neural responsiveness in the orbitofrontal cortex, medial prefrontal cortex and the amygdala. Are the contestants on Fear [and Disgust] Factor screened with such questionnaires before going on the show??

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Friday, July 21, 2006

Hypnosis and Pain Control

Now back to our irregularly scheduled neuroscience programming!

It's been so long since The Neurocritic began a plodding series on hypnosis (June 6 to be exact), that the in press articles are now in print in the Journal of Physiology (Paris). The final topic for discussion is the use of hypnosis to control pain.

Functional neuroanatomy of the hypnotic state
Pages 463-469
Marie-Elisabeth Faymonville, Mélanie Boly and Steven Laureys
SummaryPlus | Full Text + Links | PDF (283 K)

. . . Second, we looked at the anti-nociceptive effects of hypnosis. Compared to the resting state, hypnosis reduced pain perception by approximately 50%. The hypnosis-induced reduction of affective and sensory responses to noxious thermal stimulation were modulated by the activity in the midcingulate cortex (area 24a′). Finally, we assessed changes in cerebral functional connectivity related to hypnosis. Compared to normal alertness (i.e., rest and mental imagery), the hypnotic state, significantly enhanced the functional modulation between midcingulate cortex and a large neural network involved in sensory, affective, cognitive and behavioral aspects of nociception. These findings show that not only pharmacological but also psychological strategies for pain control can modulate the cerebral network involved in noxious perception.

What are some of the brain regions involved in the perception of pain? A meta-analysis of 35 imaging studies is illustrated below.






By way of explanation, back in January, I became addicted to BrainMap,
an online database of published functional neuroimaging experiments with coordinate-based (Talairach) activation locations. The goal of BrainMap is to provide a vehicle to share methods and results of brain functional imaging studies. It is a tool to rapidly retrieve and understand studies in specific research domains, such as language, memory, attention, reasoning, emotion, and perception, and to perform meta-analyses of like studies.
where one can do
coordinate-based meta-analyses according to the activation likelihood estimation (ALE) method.
That's when I did the ALE meta-analysis for imaging studies of physical pain, for comparison to "empathy for pain" and the "social rejection of being excluded from Pong" (which, of course, feels exactly like being cut open with a scalpel).
Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, Williams KD. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302: 290-2.

A neuroimaging study examined the neural correlates of social exclusion and tested the hypothesis that the brain bases of social pain are similar to those of physical pain. Participants were scanned while playing a virtual ball-tossing game in which they were ultimately excluded. Paralleling results from physical pain studies, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) was more active during exclusion than during inclusion and correlated positively with self-reported distress. Right ventral prefrontal cortex (RVPFC) was active during exclusion and correlated negatively with self-reported distress. ACC changes mediated the RVPFC-distress correlation, suggesting that RVPFC regulates the distress of social exclusion by disrupting ACC activity.
More on that topic later.

[But I think I might actually like this study -- relatively speaking -- since it doesn't equate social rejection with physical pain.
Somerville LH, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM. (2006). Anterior cingulate cortex responds differentially to expectancy violation and social rejection. Nat Neurosci. Jul 2 [Epub ahead of print]

This study investigated human anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) involvement during a task that dissociated expectancy violation from social rejection. Across two studies, participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging while making social judgments and receiving fictitious feedback that was either positive or negative and consistent or inconsistent with their expectations. The results demonstrate that the dorsal ACC is sensitive to expectancy violations, whereas the ventral ACC is differentially responsive to social feedback.]
Now back to physical pain. OK, here's another ALE map that shows clusters of activation (below). In this map, different clusters of activation are coded with different colors. The first map (above) was color-coded for significance level.



Technical details: ALE is a quantitative meta-analysis method (Turkeltaub et al., 2002) that can be used to infer function-location relationships from the functional neuroimaging literature. At the time of this analysis, BrainMap, a Java software application developed at the Research Imaging Center in San Antonio, contained 35 papers reporting activations in studies that delivered painful stimuli to the participants, and 34 of these were included in the meta-analysis. The Talairach coordinates of all pain-related activations were used to estimate voxel-wise activation likelihoods. A false discovery rate threshold of p less than .05 and a cluster extent threshold of 100 mm3 were applied to the ALE map. The resulting map identified the regions of activation common to all studies comprising the meta-analysis. Sixteen clusters were identified, with the largest being centered in the thalamus, insula, and cerebellum.

So there's evidence (Kulkarni et al., 2005) for a lateral pain system (ventral lateral nucleus of the thalamus, primary and secondary somatosensory cortices), a medial pain system (medial thalamic nuclei, anterior cingulate cortex, other prefrontal regions, and the fearful amygdala), and the intermediate insula, which sits between them. The lateral system is related to the sensory aspects of pain, while the medial is related to more "perceptual/affective" components of pain. What hypnosis does is to increase activity in the ACC, which may then downregulate activity in the sensory regions (see also Faymonville et al., 2000). 1

Would I have surgery under hypnosis?
Meurisse M, Defechereux T, Hamoir E, Maweja S, Marchettini P, Gollogly L, Degauque C, Joris J, Faymonville ME. (1999). Hypnosis with conscious sedation instead of general anaesthesia? Applications in cervical endocrine surgery. Acta Chir Belg. 99:151-8.
Uh, NO!

1 Functional connectivity analysis methods DO NOT prove causality, although some practitioners might want to believe otherwise.

References

Derbyshire SW, Whalley MG, Stenger VA, Oakley DA. (2004). Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. Neuroimage 23:392-401.

Faymonville ME, Laureys S, Degueldre C, DelFiore G, Luxen A, Franck G, Lamy M, Maquet P. (2000). Neural mechanisms of antinociceptive effects of hypnosis. Anesthesiology 92:1257-67.

Kulkarni B, Bentley DE, Elliott R, Youell P, Watson A, Derbyshire SW, Frackowiak RS, Friston KJ, Jones AK. (2005). Attention to pain localization and unpleasantness discriminates the functions of the medial and lateral pain systems. Eur J Neurosci. 21:3133-42.

Raij TT, Numminen J, Narvanen S, Hiltunen J, Hari R. (2005). Brain correlates of subjective reality of physically and psychologically induced pain. PNAS 102:2147-51.

Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF, Jones KM, Zeffiro TA (2002) Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: Method and validation.
NeuroImage 16:765-780.

ADDENDUM: Interestingly, the opposite manipulation has also been examined (Raij et al., 2004; Derbyshire et al., 2004). Hypnotically-induced pain activates the same pain matrix as real pain, but with less activity in the sensory areas.

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Thursday, July 20, 2006

Trumpet Improv With Exploding Bombs

Sometimes, Cognitive Neuroscience just doesn't seem very important.

from KERBLOG...

STARRY NIGHT (excerpt) 6.31 min

a minimalistic improvisation by:
mazen kerbaj / trumpet
the israeli air force / bombs

recorded by mazen kerbaj on the balcony of his flat in beirut,
on the night of 15th to 16th of july 2006.

© mazen kerbaj 2006

[NOTE: you have to crank up the volume to hear anything. Heartbreaking.]

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Tuesday, July 18, 2006

Chicks Dig Metaphors

A metaphor is a glorious thing
A diamond ring
The first day of summer
A metaphor is a breath of fresh air
A turn-on
An aphrodisiac

--Sparks
"Metaphor" <----- CLICK HERE

[OPEN IN NEW TAB OR WINDOW AND LISTEN WHILE READING]

Chris at Mixing Memory has reposted an excellent series on metaphor:

Metaphor I: A Brief History of Metaphors in Cognitive Science

Metaphor II: "Metaphor Is Like Analogy"

Don't don't don't don't
Don't mix them

We we we
Won't mix them

--ibid
Alternate perspectives from Cognitive Linguistics can be found here:

Gilles Fauconnier


Blending and Conceptual Integration

Conceptual Blending

And of course from the name that Chris dare not type: Lakoff

[Honestly, Chris, what do you have against the man? Never mind.]


EVERYBODY NOW!
DANCING IS REQUIRED!
Chicks dig dig
d-i-g
dig dig metaphors

--ibid

[but maybe he's getting to Cognitive Linguistics... oh look, yes he does]

Metaphor III: Metaphor Is Categorization

Use them wisely
Use them well
And you'll never know the hell
Of loneliness

--ibid

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Tuesday, July 11, 2006

A Book of Broken Rules

Out for a short break. Will be back soon. Until then, I leave you with..



Olivia Parker
A Book of Broken Rules (1994)

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Sunday, July 09, 2006

I've Walked These Streets

in a carnival
of sights to see
all the cheap thrill seekers
the vendors and the dealers
they crowded around me

"Carnival"
-Natalie Merchant


The latest issue of The Synapse (Vol. 1, n. 2) is up at Blog Around The Clock!

have I been blind
have I been lost
inside my self and
my own mind
hypnotized
mesmerized
by what my eyes have seen?

-ibid

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Wednesday, July 05, 2006

The Precuneus and Recovery From a Minimally Conscious State

You've probably heard about the case of Terry Wallis, a 42 year old man in Arkansas who spent 19 years in a "minimally conscious state" following severe traumatic brain injury. Then one day he saw his mother walk into the room and (after not speaking all those years) said "Mom."
The Aspen workgroup defined the minimally conscious state (MCS) as a condition of severely altered consciousness in which the person demonstrates minimal but definite behavioral evidence of self or environmental awareness (Giacino et al, 1997).
A number of bloggers have commented on how his case is different from that of Terry Schiavo, who was in a persistent vegetative state (not a MCS).

What I'll focus on here is the work of researchers at Cornell, who followed Mr. Wallis using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and PET scanning over an 18 month period.
Using a novel technique, they saw evidence of new growth in the midline cerebellum, an area involved in motor control, as Mr. Wallis gained strength and range in his limbs. Another area of new growth, located along the back of the brain, is believed by some experts to be a central switching center for conscious awareness.

The daily exercises, the interactions with his parents, his regular dose of antidepressant medication: any or all of these might have spurred brain cells to grow more connections, the researchers said.

"The big missed opportunity is that we didn't know this guy would spontaneously emerge, and we didn't get to monitor him before then" to find out what preceded it, Dr. Schiff said.
The "central switching center for conscious awareness" is the precuneus, interestingly enough.
Henning U. Voss, Aziz M. Uluç, Jonathan P. Dyke, Richard Watts, Erik J. Kobylarz, Bruce D. McCandliss, Linda A. Heier, Bradley J. Beattie, Klaus A. Hamacher, Shankar Vallabhajosula, Stanley J. Goldsmith, Douglas Ballon, Joseph T. Giacino and Nicholas D. Schiff. (2006). Possible axonal regrowth in late recovery from the minimally conscious state. J. Clin. Invest. 116: 2005-2011. OPEN ACCESS ARTICLE!

We used diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) to study 2 patients with traumatic brain injury. The first patient recovered reliable expressive language after 19 years in a minimally conscious state (MCS); the second had remained in MCS for 6 years. Comparison of white matter integrity in the patients and 20 normal subjects using histograms of apparent diffusion constants and diffusion anisotropy identified widespread altered diffusivity and decreased anisotropy in the damaged white matter. These findings remained unchanged over an 18-month interval between 2 studies in the first patient. In addition, in this patient, we identified large, bilateral regions of posterior white matter with significantly increased anisotropy that reduced over 18 months. In contrast, notable increases in anisotropy within the midline cerebellar white matter in the second study correlated with marked clinical improvements in motor functions. This finding was further correlated with an increase in resting metabolism measured by PET in this subregion. Aberrant white matter structures were evident in the second patient’s DTI images but were not clinically correlated. We propose that axonal regrowth may underlie these findings and provide a biological mechanism for late recovery. Our results are discussed in the context of recent experimental studies that support this inference.
In a commentary on the article, Laureys, Boly, and Maquet note the importance of the precuneus in conscious awareness:
The most remarkable finding in the Voss et al. study (12) was the MRI assessment of transiently increased fractional anisotropy and directionality in the posterior midline cortices (encompassing the cuneus and precuneus), interpreted as increased myelinated fiber densities and novel corticocortical sprouting, paralleling the emergence of the patient from MCS. The same area of the patient’s brain also showed amplified metabolic activity, as measured by PET. This finding stresses the importance of the posterior medial structures in consciousness of self and interaction with the environment (14, 15). Activity in the medial parietal cortex (i.e., precuneus) seems to show it to be the brain region that best differentiates MCS from VS patients (16). Interestingly, this area is among the most active brain regions in conscious waking (15) and is among the least active in altered states of consciousness, such as pharmacological coma (17), sleep (18), dementia (19), Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome, and postanoxic amnesia (20). It has been suggested that this richly connected multimodal posteromedial associative area is part of the neural network subserving human awareness (21).
The figure above illustrates Mr. Wallis' brain. On the left is a DTI scan to trace fiber tracts. On the right is a PET scan to measure glucose metabolism. The areas highlighted are on the posterior medial surface of the brain, the parietal-occipital region, which includes the cuneus and precuneus.

It's really interesting to compare the DTI scan of Mr. Wallis with that of a control subject, as illustrated below. The first thing to note is the degeneration in Patient 1's corpus callosum (the big red crescent in the middle of the control's brain, labeled cc). This structure is a huge white matter tract that connects the two cerebral hemispheres. But what's most novel is the unique white matter tract in Patient 1's parietal-occipital region. Could this presumed axonal growth be the basis of the remarkable improvement in Mr. Wallis' state of awareness?


Steven Laureys, Mélanie Boly and Pierre Maquet (2006). Tracking the recovery of consciousness from coma. J. Clin. Invest. 116:1823-1825. OPEN ACCESS ARTICLE!

Predicting the chances of recovery of consciousness and communication in patients who survive their coma but transit in a vegetative state or minimally conscious state (MCS) remains a major challenge for their medical caregivers. Very few studies have examined the slow neuronal changes underlying functional recovery of consciousness from severe chronic brain damage. A case study in this issue of the JCI reports an extraordinary recovery of functional verbal communication and motor function in a patient who remained in MCS for 19 years (see the related article beginning on page 2005). Diffusion tensor MRI showed increased fractional anisotropy (assumed to reflect myelinated fiber density) in posteromedial cortices, encompassing cuneus and precuneus. These same areas showed increased glucose metabolism as studied by PET scanning, likely reflecting the neuronal regrowth paralleling the patient’s clinical recovery. This case shows that old dogmas need to be oppugned, as recovery with meaningful reduction in disability continued in this case for nearly 2 decades after extremely severe traumatic brain injury.

photo: Ron Phillips for The New York Times

Terry Wallis at his home in Arkansas.

Mute 19 Years, He Helps Reveal Brain's Mysteries

. . .

He does not feel any physical pain, he told his parents, and he has no real sense of time. He also said recently that he was "proud" to be alive.

"It is good to know all that," said his father, sitting on the porch on Saturday evening.

"It's good to hear him say that, because if he didn't say so, you'd just have no way to know."

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Monday, July 03, 2006

Carnival of Sorts

Encephalon - A neuroscience carnival

First edition available on
The neurophilosopher's blog.

Thanks to MC for inviting me to participate.


Carnival Of Sorts (Box Cars)

From: Chronic Town

There's a secret stigma, reaping wheel.
Diminish, a carnival of sorts.
Chronic town, poster torn, reaping wheel.
Stranger, stranger to these parts.

Gentlemen don't get caught, cages under cage. Gentlemen don't get caught,
Boxcars (are pulling) out of town. Boxcars (are pulling) out of town.
Boxcars (are pulling) out of town.

--R.E.M.

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Whitney Diane Sawyer Skit



The brilliant Debra Wilson as Whitney Houston.

Fast forward to 2:30 to hear the famous lines.

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

Crack Is Found in Shuttle's Foam Insulation



Headline in today's New York Times. I formed this image (above) and had a brief chuckle.

I had an even longer laugh when recalling the hysterical Mad TV skit where Debra Wilson (as Whitney Houston), in a mock interview with Diane Sawyer, says the following:
I don't do crack.
cuz crack is cheap
crack is cheap, OK
crack is wack!
I do freebase now.




Oh, life would be so much easier if I could get one of these $450,000 blogger grants from the US Air Force.

[from Follow Me Here.]

Subscribe to Post Comments [Atom]

eXTReMe Tracker